Ir al contenido principal

TEMPORARY PREFERENCE, RISK AND NEUROFINANCE

In the world of financial decisions (investment, savings, intertemporal consumption, etc.), the emotional component of decision-making plays a crucial role, despite the widespread belief that financial markets are plenty of thoughtful cost-benefit analysis, and complex, careful and rational expectations predictive models. Let's see some topics of interest in Finance, but nuanced from the point of view of modern Cognitive Neurosciences.

Temporal Preferences in the Brain

To study and model intertemporal decisions, traditional economics has generally used the theory of discounted utility, based on the idea that economic agents prefer a similar reward more if it is obtained in the present than in the future; and similarly, future costs would be less painful than the costs to be faced today.

To illustrate, empirical evidence suggests that if a person is given a choice of $ 100 now or $ 110 tomorrow, he generally prefers $ 100 now, while the same person if asked to select between $ 100 in two years or $ 110 in two years and one day, it could get to prefer $ 110 in two years and one day without any problem. It seems then that discount rates tend to be higher in the short term than in the long term.

From the neuro point of view, then the following questions arise:
  • what is the mechanism behind these intertemporal decisions?
  • do they arise from a single preference mechanism or from multiple systems that interact?

In seeking to answer these questions, Samuel McLure, David Laibson, George Loewenstein and Jonathan Cohen[1], using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), examined the neural correlate of time discount while subjects made choices among monetary rewards options that varied across time.

The experiment consisted of giving the participants a choice between a sum in the short term and another in the long term, the first being less than the second. Both options were separated by a minimum time lag of two weeks, and in some pairs of options, the earliest option was immediately available. The hypothesis was that the behavior pattern of the two traditional parameters (β and δ) of the intertemporal discount function arises from the joint influence of different neural processes.

The β related to the limbic system and the δ related to the lateral prefrontal cortex and other structures associated with higher cognitive functions (the more rational ones). What results did the researchers obtain? Basically, there would be two systems involved in such intertemporal decisions:
  • ·     parts of the limbic system (emotional zone of the brain) associated with the dopamine system of the central brain, including the paralimbic cortex, which would be triggered by decisions involving immediately available rewards;
  • ·      regions of the lateral prefrontal cortex and the posterior parietal cortex (eminently rational areas of the brain), uniformly involved in intertemporal decisions independently of the delay in time.


This neuroeconomic finding is consistent with the evidence that consumers act impatient today but prefer to act patients in the future, also supporting the hypothesis that different neuronal systems are activated by intertemporal decisions: the impatience of the short term, which is driven by the limbic system (emotional, not deliberative), and that responds preferably to immediate rewards and to a lesser extent to future rewards; and long-term patience, dominated by the lateral prefrontal cortex and associated structures (the most deliberative parts of our brain), which can rationally evaluate exchanges between abstract rewards, including rewards over longer periods.

Neurotransmitters of Financial Risk

In the securities markets it is very often observed investors cut profits quickly, but that they endure losses very much. This is because we humans have an innate tendency to avoid loss. We develop a series of brain mechanisms to do everything possible to minimize them. We tend to mobilize much more with the fact of avoiding loss than with the achievement of a profit.

The Neurosciences show that certain regions of the brain, involved with the reward systems, are crucial to forget (or diminish) the pain of financial losses. It is believed that a neurotransmitter, norepinephrine, is essential for the response to loss of money. Men with high levels of noradrenaline are less sensitive to pain in the face of financial losses.

Another important neurotransmitter, dopamine, also plays a crucial role in taking financial risks. Several scientific studies find that increasing the levels of dopamine in healthy adults leads to choosing investment / betting options that are riskier than the average.


Also recent neuroscientific research has revealed the possibility that the endogenous hormones of stock market agents, in particular, testosterone and cortisol, can critically affect financial decision-making in markets.

Recent research shows that cortisol, a hormone that modulates the response to physical or psychological stress, predicts instability in financial markets. In a particular study[2], samples of salivary levels of cortisol and testosterone were taken from people who participated in a controlled experiment (142 sample people), and it was found that the individual and combined levels of endogenous cortisol predict subsequent risk assumption and instability of market prices.

In summary, from the point of view of Neurofinances, understanding the role of certain neurotransmitters and / or brain areas, both in intertemporal decision-making and in the predisposition to assume excessive risks, will seem to play a predominant role in the next years, with the idea of ​​training the brain in order to improve the investment performance, and not to reach extreme situations of destabilization and volatility as they are often seen.

Author: Sebastián Laza (MBA, Neuroeconomist)




[1] Mc Lure, Samuel, Laibson David, Loewenstein George and Cohen Jonathan, Separate Neural System Value Inmediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards, Science, October 2004.
[2] Cueva, Carlos; Roberts, R Edward; Spencer, Tom; Rani, Nisha; Tempest, Michelle; Tobler, Philippe N; erbert, Joe; Rustichini, Aldo; Cortisol and Testosterone Increase Financial Risk Taking and May Destabilize Markets. http://www.zora.uzh.ch/id/eprint/113359/2/cortisol%20and%20testosterone%20supporting%20material.pdf

Comentarios

Entradas populares de este blog

LO MISMO PERO DIFERENTE: EL EFECTO ENMARQUE

El efecto enmarque es uno de los tantos sesgos cognitivos de nuestra mente, humana y poco racional, en el que el cerebro toma decisiones sobre determinada información según cómo se le presenta la misma. Dicho efecto se usa a menudo en el marketing para influir en los responsables de las decisiones y en las compras, aprovechando la tendencia de las personas a ver la misma información, pero responder a ella de diferentes maneras, dependiendo de si una opción específica se presenta en un marco positivo o en un marco negativo.  ¿Cuál de estos productos elegiría: uno presentado como con 95% de efectividad, o el mismo con un 5% de falla? La mayoría de las personas es más probable que elija la primera opción, aunque las dos opciones sean idénticas. El modelo económico estándar predice que las personas siempre tomarán la misma decisión si se les muestran los mismos resultados, al maximizar la utilidad esperada. En su innovador estudio de 1979, Amos Tversky y Daniel Kahnemann, ambos po

Marcadores Somáticos: Atajos para la Toma de Decisiones

La hipótesis del marcador somático, de la mano de Antonio Damasio, ha sido muy relevante al momento de comprender el papel que juega la emoción en la toma de decisiones. La idea consiste en que las consecuencias de una decisión producen en la persona una determinada reacción emocional que es subjetiva, que se puede “vivenciar”, y que a su vez es somática, es decir se traduce en reacciones musculares, neuroendócrinas o neurofisiológicas. Esta respuesta emocional a su vez se puede asociar con consecuencias, ya sean negativas o positivas, que se repiten con cierta constancia en el tiempo y que provoquen dicha respuesta. Este mecanismo de asociación es el que produce lo que Damasio llama “marcador somático” y que influye en las decisiones a tomar a futuro. De esta manera, la reacción emotiva pasada influye en la toma de decisiones futura, posibilitando la anticipación de las consecuencias y guiando el proceso de resolución final. En este sentido se afirma que los marcadores

UN MUNDO DE GENTE APURADA

¿Se han puesto a pensar por qué andamos por la vida tan apurados? Dormimos poco, comemos apurados, compramos apurados, manejamos apurados, estudiamos apurados, multitasking en la oficina, zapping permanente en tv, en la música del auto, etc. VAMOS A EXPLOTAR. Se nos pasa el año volando, los días volando, las horas… Es frenético el ritmo.  ¿Pero quién nos apura? Los economistas decimos que “la gente prefiere ir más rápido o más lento, es decir elegir más a corto o a largo plazo, en función de lo que llamamos  tasa de preferencia temporal. Y está comprobado que,  en promedio, la gente suele valorar más obtener recompensa ahora, aunque sea menor, que esperar un tiempo Y OBTENER ALGO MAYOR A FUTURO.  No queremos esperar… lo queremos todo ya.  Podés legir rendir para un 10, pero te querés sacar de encima la materia ya, estudiás menos y aprobás con un 7.  Podés elegir esperar una semana, comparar precios y modelos, y comprarte el teléfono móvil nuevo … pero no… te en